Your browser does not support JavaScript or JavaScript has been turned off.
 
Steven J. Haggbloom, Ph.D.
WKU Psych Home
WKU Home


 

 

 

100 Most Eminent Psychologists of the 20th Century. Haggbloom et al., Review of general Psychology, 2002

Revised 05/01/09

Table 4  as it appeared in Haggbloom et al. (2002). The 100 Most Eminent Psychologists of the 20th Century. Review of General Psychology, 6, 139-152. (pdf format)

Rank-ordered list of 99 of the 100 most-eminent psychologists of the 20th century

Alphabetical list of the 99 of the100 most-eminent psychologists of the 20th century

Revised Table 4     |     Errata

Rank-ordered list of 99 of the 100 most-eminent psychologists of the 20th century modified on the basis of the errata that appear below and Black, S.L. (2003) Cannonical [sic] confusions, an illusory allusion, and more: A critique of Haggbloom et al.'s list of eminent psychologists (2002). Psychological Reports, 92, 853-857.

Alphabetical list of 99 of the 100 most-eminent psychologists of the 20th century modified on the basis of the errata that appear below and Black, S.L. (2003) Cannonical [sic] confusions, an illusory allusion, and more: A critique of Haggbloom et al.'s list of eminent psychologists (2002). Psychological Reports, 92, 853-857.

A version of Table 4 with annotated information about each entry

Jews among the 100 most eminent psychologists

Article in the APA Monitor

Poster: Tumasjan, A., Männich, M. & Spörrle, M. (2008). The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century on the internet: Do internet page counts provide latent indicators of scientific eminence? [Abstract]. International Journal of Psychology, 43(3/4), 122.

Abstract: Recently, Haggbloom et al. (2002) established a rank-ordered list of the 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century meticulously measured by several quantitative and qualitative indicators. We aimed at replicating this listing by simply using page counts obtained from three major internet search engines using different search queries and five points of measurements. The resulting highly reliable indicators of internet frequency were consistently positively associated with the existing ranking and this correlation reached significance when the field of research was included in the query. We conclude that frequency data obtained by this method can be considered a simple and valid indicator of scientific impact and discuss additional applications of this method.

UK Internet Survey published in The Psychologist

Elliot Aronson on being Ranked 78th

Journal Citation List: Rank-ordered    |    Alphabetical 

Textbook Citation List: Rank ordered   |  Alphabetical

Survey List:  Rank-ordered   |   Alphabetical

From Haggbloom et al. (2002). The 100 Most Eminent Psychologists of the 20th Century. Review of General Psychology, 6, 139-152.

and

Errata. (2003). Review of General Psychology, 7, 37.

I note two errors in Haggbloom et al's list of eminent psychologists. One is the appearance of the name of W. Gary Cannon, a recently-deceased psychologist, who was neither a member of the National Academy of Sciences (NAS) nor associated with the Cannon-Bard theory of emotion. These were instead the accomplishments of the great American physiologist, Walter B. Cannon, who made important contributions to psychology. A second error involving the name of Cannon concerns the entry for an individual identified as Margarete [sic] Washburn whose accomplishments were those of Margaret F. Washburn. Haggbloom et al credit her with an eponym for the Cannon-Washburn experiment on the nature of hunger sensations (Cannon, W.B., & Washburn, A.L., 1912, An explanation of hunger. American Journal of Physiology, 29, 441-454.). The Washburn of this famous experiment was A.L. Washburn, and the eponym memorializes his heroic contribution to this experiment, not Margaret F. Washburn's. Consequently, a reassessment and possible revision of the list of Haggbloom et al (2002) is called for. From: Stephen L. Black, Department of Psychology, Bishop's University, Lennoxville, QC J1M-1Z7, Canada..

The authors thank Stephen L. Black, Bishop's University, for identifying our errors and especially for drawing our attention to the mis-attributed Cannon-Washburn eponym. In Table 4 on page 147, the name W. Gary Cannon should in fact be W. B. Cannon. Also, the eponym "Cannon-Washburn experiment" was incorrectly attributed to Margaret Washburn rather than A. L. Washburn. Correcting this error (a) removes Margaret Washburn from the list, (b), moves each name below Washburn up one place, and (c) moves Leo Postman from #100 (previously unidentified) to #99. Postman has a ranking of 52 on the Journal Citation List and was elected to the National Academy of Sciences. From: Steven J. Haggbloom, Department of Psychology, Western Kentucky University, 1 Big Red Way, Bowling Green, KY 42101.

Black (2003) identified two additional errors, not noted in the errata published in the Review of general Psychology, that affected the positions of individuals on the most-eminent list. (See Black, S.L. (2003) Cannonical [sic] confusions, an illusory allusion, and more: A critique of Haggbloom et al.'s list of eminent psychologists (2002). Psychological Reports, 92, 853-857.) 

  • An eponym, the "Deutsch illusion," was erroneously attributed to Morton Deutsch. That eponym should be credited to Diana Deutsch. Removing the credit for this eponym moves Morton Deutsch from a position of 63rd on the most-eminent list to 78th.
  • An eponym,  "Hall's theory of interpersonal zones," was misattributed to G. Stanley Hall. That eponym should be credited to anthropologist Edward T. Hall. Removing the credit for this eponym has the effect of removing G. Stanley Hall from the most-eminent list.

The removal from the most-eminent list of Hall and of Margaret Washburn created two "slots" at the bottom of the list that are now occupied, respectively, by Leo Postman and B.J. Winer.

Other errors identified by Black (2003) had no effect on rankings in the most-eminent list. Note especially that correcting the error in the number of citations attributed to Eysenck on the textbook citation list did not alter his position on the most-eminent list because there was a relatively large composite score gap between Eysenk at position 13 and Erikson at position 12. 


Revised 05/01/09